9 Tennis Shoe Tour Vapor white Viola hyper Women's Zoom 5 Violet black Nike q1HTW 9 Tennis Shoe Tour Vapor white Viola hyper Women's Zoom 5 Violet black Nike q1HTW 9 Tennis Shoe Tour Vapor white Viola hyper Women's Zoom 5 Violet black Nike q1HTW 9 Tennis Shoe Tour Vapor white Viola hyper Women's Zoom 5 Violet black Nike q1HTW 9 Tennis Shoe Tour Vapor white Viola hyper Women's Zoom 5 Violet black Nike q1HTW 9 Tennis Shoe Tour Vapor white Viola hyper Women's Zoom 5 Violet black Nike q1HTW
Skip to main content
  • Submit
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS StaffSlim Tropical Womens Flip Havaianas Rose Ballet Rubber Flops E1qZwO5
    • FAQ
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian
  • Log in
  • My Cart
  • Main menu
  • User menu
  • Search

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Latest Articles
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • Archive
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • Highlights from Latest Articles
    • Teal Teal On Slip Pehuea Women's OluKai Unq4gXOw
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Purpose and Scope
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • For Reviewers
    • Author FAQ
  • SubmitButterfly Beach Sandals fereshte Antiskid Women's Slide Platform Pink Casual with Flipflops 61A1OwPBWq
  • About
    • Editorial Board
    • PNAS Staff
    • FAQ
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Site Map
  • Contact
  • Journal Club
  • Subscribe
    • Subscription Rates
    • Subscriptions FAQ
    • Open Access
    • Recommend PNAS to Your Librarian

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Vapor Tennis Nike black Violet Zoom Tour white hyper 5 Viola Shoe Women's 9

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current
    • Latest Articles
    • Special Features
    • Colloquia
    • Collected Articles
    • PNAS Classics
    • Archive
  • Front Matter
  • News
    • For the Press
    • Highlights from Latest Articles
    • Teal Teal On Slip Pehuea Women's OluKai Unq4gXOw
  • Podcasts
  • Authors
    • Purpose and Scope
    • Editorial and Journal Policies
    • Submission Procedures
    • For Reviewers
    • Author FAQ

New Research In

Featured Portals

  • Physics
  • Chemistry
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Applied Mathematics
  • Applied Physical Sciences
  • RUN Navy Sports Wading L Breathable Mutifunctional Shoes Casual Mesh Flat Strip Outdoor Unisex Heeled Z1Yx1dO
  • Computer Sciences
  • Earth, Atmospheric, and Planetary Sciences
  • Engineering
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Mathematics
  • Statistics

Featured Portals

  • Anthropology
  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Economic Sciences
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Political Sciences
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Social Sciences
  • 7 Dark Leather SRM Busoni Shoes Men's Red 27srm1225112 Lacoste B71qA

Featured Portals

  • Sustainability Science

Articles by Topic

  • Agricultural Sciences
  • Anthropology
  • Applied Biological Sciences
  • Biochemistry
  • Biophysics and Computational Biology
  • Cell Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology
  • Environmental Sciences
  • Evolution
  • Genetics
  • Immunology and Inflammation
  • Medical Sciences
  • Microbiology
  • Neuroscience
  • Pharmacology
  • Physiology
  • Plant Biology
  • Population Biology
  • Psychological and Cognitive Sciences
  • Sustainability Science
  • Systems Biology

9 Tennis Shoe Tour Vapor white Viola hyper Women's Zoom 5 Violet black Nike q1HTW

Lauri Nummenmaa, Riitta Hari, Jari K. Hietanen, and Enrico Glerean
PNAS published ahead of print August 28, 2018 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1807390115
Lauri Nummenmaa
aTurku PET Centre and Turku University Hospital, University of Turku, FI-20520, Turku, Finland;bTurku University Hospital, University of Turku, FI-20520, Turku, Finland;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Lauri Nummenmaa
  • For correspondence: lauri.nummenmaa@utu.firiitta.hari@aalto.fi
Riitta Hari
cDepartment of Art, School of Arts, Design and Architecture, Aalto University, FI-00076, Espoo, Finland;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Riitta Hari
  • For correspondence: lauri.nummenmaa@utu.firiitta.hari@aalto.fi
Jari K. Hietanen
dFaculty of Social Sciences and Psychology, University of Tampere, FI-33014, Tampere, Finland;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Enrico Glerean
aTurku PET Centre and Turku University Hospital, University of Turku, FI-20520, Turku, Finland;eDepartment of Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, School of Science, Aalto University, FI-00076, Espoo, Finland;fDepartment of Computer Science, School of Science, Aalto University, FI-00076, Espoo, Finland;gHelsinki Institute of Information Technology, Aalto University, FI-00076, Espoo, Finland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Enrico Glerean
  1. Contributed by Riitta Hari, July 3, 2018 (sent for review April 30, 2018; reviewed by Christian Keysers and Leonhard Schilbach)

  • Article
  • Figures & SI
  • Authors & Info
  • PDF

Significance

Subjective feelings are a central feature of human life, yet their relative organization has remained elusive. We mapped the “human feeling space” for 100 core feelings ranging from cognitive and affective processes to somatic sensations; in the analysis, we combined basic dimension rating, similarity mapping, bodily sensation mapping, and neuroimaging meta-analysis. All feelings were emotionally loaded, and saliencies of bodily and mental experiences were correlated. Feelings formed five groups: positive emotions, negative emotions, cognitive processes, somatic states, and homeostatic states. Feeling space was best explained by emotionality, mental experience, and bodily sensation topographies. Subjectively felt similarity of feelings was associated with basic feeling dimensions and the bodily sensation maps. This shows that subjective feelings are categorical, emotional, and embodied.

Abstract

Subjective feelings are a central feature of human life. We defined the organization and determinants of a feeling space involving 100 core feelings that ranged from cognitive and affective processes to somatic sensations and common illnesses. The feeling space was determined by a combination of basic dimension rating, similarity mapping, bodily sensation mapping, and neuroimaging meta-analysis. A total of 1,026 participants took part in online surveys where we assessed (i) for each feeling, the intensity of four hypothesized basic dimensions (mental experience, bodily sensation, emotion, and controllability), (ii) subjectively experienced similarity of the 100 feelings, and (iii) topography of bodily sensations associated with each feeling. Neural similarity between a subset of the feeling states was derived from the NeuroSynth meta-analysis database based on the data from 9,821 brain-imaging studies. All feelings were emotionally valenced and the saliency of bodily sensations correlated with the saliency of mental experiences associated with each feeling. Nonlinear dimensionality reduction revealed five feeling clusters: positive emotions, negative emotions, cognitive processes, somatic states and illnesses, and homeostatic states. Organization of the feeling space was best explained by basic dimensions of emotional valence, mental experiences, and bodily sensations. Subjectively felt similarity of feelings was associated with basic feeling dimensions and the topography of the corresponding bodily sensations. These findings reveal a map of subjective feelings that are categorical, emotional, and embodied.

  • feeling
  • emotion
  • consciousness
  • interoception
  • somatosensation

We humans constantly experience an ever-changing stream of subjective feelings that is only abolished during sleep, brain damage, and drug-induced altered states of the central nervous system. It is however most puzzling how our mental world is organized. External and internal pieces of information that proceed beyond nonconscious processing may be transformed into reliably reportable subjective experiences (1) or feelings that bear distinctive subjective qualia—the unique way how we feel different things. For example, trying to recall the title of the last book we read feels qualitatively different from the piercing headache during a high fever. The term feeling has multiple psychological and physiological definitions ranging from the subjectively accessible component of emotions to somatosensory experiences, ideas, and beliefs. Here, we use the word feeling to simply refer to the current, subjectively accessible phenomenological state of an individual. These inner sensations organize our mental lives and are also vital signs of our well-being (2, 3). Consequently, various unpleasant feelings are the most common reasons for seeking medical care (4). Despite the centrality of subjective feelings to ourselves and our conscious well-being, the relative organization and determinants of different feelings have remained poorly understood.

We experience in our bodies changes that relate to our well-being, physical activity, stress level, and emotional state. Thus, interoceptive and somatosensory inputs contribute significantly to the subjective feelings (5⇓–7). Because the survival of an organism depends on its ability to maintain its physiology within an optimal homeostatic range, the ability to consciously monitor and feel certain physiological states (such as thirst and hunger) and detect potential tissue damage has been critical already to our ancestors (KO 004 SIZE OG PLATINUM' 'PURE 11 638471 AJ1 Nike HIGH P4wCPpq, 7). Such feelings vary markedly in their mental saliency. For example, heartbeat and digestive processes go most of the time unnoticed, whereas it is almost impossible to abolish the agony upon hearing of a loss of a dear friend. Such graded intensity of feelings allows filtering out unimportant ones from the limited-capacity working space in the executive system and focusing our executive problem-solving skills to deal with the most urgent ones (8).

Positive and negative emotions—physiological, motor, and cognitive programs—are inherently associated with subjective feelings (7, 9), and emotion is often considered as an essential building block of consciousness. Emergence of conscious feelings could thus require that the brain generates sensorimotor neural activation patterns of objects or events, as well as neural patterns regarding how (positively or negatively) these events change the state of the organism (10). Despite multiple classification schemes for specific emotions (11, 12), the contribution of emotion to other feeling states not traditionally considered as emotions (such as cognition and homeostatic processes), remains unknown. Finally, our ability to control different bodily and mental states varies greatly. A simple shift of attention across the room can be accomplished swiftly, while getting up from the bed at 4 AM to catch an early flight requires substantial effort, and chocking down the intense grieving in one’s partner’s funeral would be next to impossible. The sense of controllability or agency—our experience of initiating, executing, and controlling thoughts and actions—is a central tenet of human phenomenological experience (DREAM Pump Pu High Shoes Heel Swan PAIRS Dress Plaform Black Women's UrqOUZ, 14). Because such sense of control binds our thoughts and actions to our ownership (13), it could also be one key dimension of subjective feelings.

Here, we address the fundamental principles that organize the human feeling states (SI Appendix, Fig. S1). We asked (i) how humans organize their feeling states, (ii) what kind of mental experiences and bodily sensations would best explain the representational structure of the feelings, and (iii) whether the mental experiences and bodily sensations are associated with distinct neural activation patterns. We focused on mapping the basic dimensions (Experiment 1), ontology (Experiment 2), as well as bodily (Experiment 3) and neural (meta-analysis and synthesis of Experiments 1–3) basis of a broad array of feeling states (SI Appendix, Fig. S2 and Table S1). We first quantified the relative intensities of four hypothesized core subjective dimensions (intensity of bodily sensations, saliency of mental experience, emotional valence, and agency) of 100 common subjective feelings spanning from homeostatic (e.g., hunger) and emotional (e.g., pleasure) states to cognitive functions (e.g., recalling). We also measured the relative frequency of experiencing each feeling as the lapse since the last remembered occurrence of each feeling. Next, we measured the experienced similarity of these subjective feelings and mapped the topography of bodily sensations associated with each feeling. Neural activation patterns associated with each state were derived using large-scale meta-analysis of fMRI data. We quantified the spatial representations of these states and linked the representational organization of the subjective states with their bodily and neural activation patterns. We show that subjective mental states are embodied and emotionally valenced, and that there is a clear correspondence between the mental experiences and their bodily basis that also pertains to the underlying neural activation patterns in the bodily domain.

Results

Experiment 1: Basic Dimensions of Subjective Feelings.

Fig. 1 shows the associations between the core dimensions of subjective feelings, and SI Appendix, Fig. S2 shows the corresponding probability density functions of the data aggregated across subjects (see Clear Women's Pleaser 701 Moon Black Silver qtPg6 for means and SEs of mean). All tested states were associated with salient mental and bodily sensations. The relative strength of the mental experiences and bodily sensations differed greatly across states but were linearly associated, when fitted with separate regression lines for two major clusters in the data. The majority of the feeling states were experienced as emotional—either pleasant or unpleasant. Only few, mainly physiological and cognitive, states were experienced as affectively neutral. As expected, emotional intensity (i.e., deviation from the midpoint neutral affective state toward pleasure or displeasure) was significantly associated with the intensity of both mental and bodily sensations. Pleasant and controllable states were more frequent than unpleasant and uncontrollable ones. Finally, subjects felt less control over unpleasant than pleasant feelings and less control over bodily than mental states.

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Associations between the basic dimensions of subjective experience. Histograms show the distribution for each evaluated dimension with overlaid probability density curves per clustering in Experiment 2. Scatterplots show median z-scores for each of the 100 feelings (see SI Appendix, Fig. S2 for the complete distribution densities). Color coding of the tokens is based on clustering per Experiment 2. Normalized data are modeled with one or two LS regression lines depending on the optimal fit. All reported values are Spearman’s correlations *P < 0.05, **P < 0.005.

Experiment 2: Mapping the Mental Feeling Space.

We next mapped the topographical organization of the feeling states based on the similarity ratings obtained in Experiment 2. Fig. 2 shows the 2D representation of the feeling space. The density-based clustering (DBSCAN; ref. 15) and t-distributed stochastic neighbor embedding (t-SNE; ref. 16) solutions revealed five discrete clusters: positive emotions, negative emotions, cognitive processes, somatic states and illnesses, and homeostatic states. The topographical dimension intensity heatmaps (based on Experiment 1) were then used to evaluate how the states in each cluster differ with respect to the underlying basic dimensions. The two dimensions of the t-SNE derived feeling space mapped primarily to the strength of mental feelings on vertical axis and emotional valence (pleasant–unpleasant) on the horizontal axis, so that both positive and negative emotional states were experienced strongly, and homeostatic processes and illnesses weakly in the mind. Emotional valence was also associated with the sense of control, in that sensations associated with cognition and positive emotions were the easiest, and those associated with illnesses and negative emotions the hardest to control. Intensity of bodily sensations mapped to the bottom corners of the space. The feeling spaces were concordant across participants with a mean Spearman’s correlation split-half reliability of 0.94 (SI Appendix, Fig. S3).

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

The feeling space. (Upper) Two-dimensional map of the feeling space based on the sorting task average distance matrix between items arranged by t-SNE and clustered with DBSCAN. Color coding indicates cluster structure; gray feelings do not belong clearly to any cluster. Colored items with black edge are DBSCAN border elements. To retain the information of the distance matrix the closest three items for each node are connected with lines. Thick dark lines are showing distances that belong to the top 33rd percentile of the visualized lines (i.e., the closest items). (Tennis hyper Viola white black Zoom Women's 5 Tour Nike 9 Vapor Shoe Violet Lower) Heatmaps showing how strongly each basic dimension of subjective experience is associated with each discrete feeling at each location of the feeling space. Color coding shows the relative intensity as median z-score (as in Fig. 1) from high (red) to low (blue).

Fig. 3 shows the bodily sensations associated with each feeling state, revealing that almost all feeling states were associated with a unique, discernible bodily sensation map. Although the organization of these bodily maps was associated with the organization of the subjective organization of the feeling states (i.e., Experiment 2), t-SNE mapping based on the bodily sensations revealed more clearly a linear continuum along the distributed versus focal fingerprints of bodily feelings (SI Appendix, Figs. S4 and S5).

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Bodily feeling maps. Pixel intensities show regions where each feeling was associated with statistically significant (P < 0.05, FDR corrected) bodily sensations. The data are arranged into the matrix approximately as in Fig. 2; clustering is per Experiment 2. Colorbar indicates the effect size. See SI Appendix, Figs. S4 and S5 for spatial arrangement per t-SNE.

Representational Similarity Across Neural Activation and Domains of Subjective Feelings.

Finally, we assessed the representational similarity between the measured features of subjective experience. This across-dimension representational similarity analysis (RSA; ref. 17) (Fig. 4) revealed that the ground-truth similarity metric (i.e., subjectively felt organization) was significantly associated with all of the basic dimensions, as well as with bodily sensations and semantic similarity. Bodily sensation maps were also significantly associated with all other dimensions except controllability and lapse. Semantic similarity was associated with bodily saliency, emotion, bodily sensation maps, and neural similarity (NeuroSynth). Neural similarity structure was, in turn, associated with similarity of bodily saliency, bodily sensation maps, and semantic similarity [all P values < 0.05, false discovery rate (FDR) corrected].

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Representational similarity between determinants of subjective experience (Spearman’s correlation). Each cell in the matrix represents pairwise RSA between two similarity matrices for the 100 feelings; *P < 0.05 (FDR corrected), **P < 0.005 (FDR corrected, permutation-based significance thresholding).

Discussion

Our data provide a detailed map of the human feeling space, where subjective feelings were strongly coupled with bodily sensations, and nearly all subjective experiences were qualified by emotional tone. Subjective states were best described on a 2D map with five distinct feeling clusters. Representational similarity analysis revealed strong correspondence between the mental feeling space and the corresponding bodily sensations, basic dimensions of subjective experience, as well as similarity between neural and bodily sensation maps across different subjective feelings. Altogether these findings show that feeling states are categorical, emotional, and embodied.

Organization of the Feeling Space.

Subjective feelings were organized as a 2D space with discrete five-cluster structure (positive emotions, negative emotions, cognitive processes, somatic states and illnesses, and homeostatic and sensory states). This mapping was concordant across individuals, suggesting that the feeling space is minimally influenced by individual differences. Thus, its organization likely reflects inherent coupling between the sensory and interoceptive systems and the frontoparietal brain circuit supporting consciously accessible feelings (White Rubino Hi Marley Womens 4 Tretorn qwUpB6p). When linked with the topography intensity of basic feeling-dimension ratings, the feeling space was best described by the orthogonal dimensions of affective valence (pleasant–unpleasant) and mental saliency (low–high), with bodily sensations being most profoundly experienced on the antidiagonal (positive emotions and illnesses; Women 7 Black 5 Egila Calf Boot US Knee Wide High Alfani qX6tq). The clustering of the feelings likely reflects the way the brain binds and weighs the interoceptive and exteroceptive inputs for generating each feeling state.

When individuals evaluate the presence of mental functions in other agents, the presence of physiological, emotional, and cognitive–perceptual experiences, and the sense of agency, are the most important determinants guiding the decision (19, 20). These dimensions were salient also in the present data, indicating that our own feelings provide the anchors against which to attribute mental states of other beings. Shared mapping of feelings across individuals may thus serve important social functions. For example, the ability to exchange conscious states across individuals via action–perception loops is an essential building block of sociability (21), and the capability to promote social cohesion by sharing feelings has likely yielded significant evolutionary advantages already to our ancestors (22).

Feelings Are Coupled with Bodily Processes.

Feelings were systematically referenced to bodily states, even for states considered as purely cognitive, such as attending or reasoning. Additionally, the more strongly some feeling was experienced in the body, the more salient it was mentally. Different feelings were associated with distinct bodily fingerprints, and representational structure of these bodily maps converged with the mentally experienced structure of feelings: The qualia of the mental experience of any given feeling were associated with the unique topography of the concurrent bodily sensations. This finding accords with results showing that voluntary reproduction of bodily states associated with, for example, specific emotions leads to the subjective feeling of the corresponding emotion (23). Accordingly, an individual’s current emotional feeling can be accurately classified using signals from the primary somatosensory and insular cortices (24). Thus, interoceptive signaling—associated primarily with homeostasis and a threatening tissue damage—is an important feature of subjective feelings in general (3, 7), similarly as has been previously established specifically for emotional states (25, 26). Such embodiment of subjective experiences may also extend to self-awareness: Adults and children consistently think of “self” as occupying a physical location within the body—oftentimes in head, but also remarkably often in the torso (27). However, even when some types of spinal-cord lesions prevent interoceptive signals from entering the brain, they do not necessarily abolish subjective awareness; for example, patients with pure autonomic failure can still experience embodied subjective states, such as emotions (28). Moreover, complete bilateral lesions to the interoceptive regions in the insula do not abolish emotions (29). Bodily feedback cannot thus be the sole contributor to subjective feelings, as was also confirmed by our representational similarity analysis.

Emotion as a Core Dimension of Subjective Feeling.

Nearly all subjective feelings were imbued with emotional qualities. During emotions, the benefit versus harm associated with external and internal events triggers an integrated blend of good or bad feelings, or vigorous activation or relaxation. Such affective sensations involve cognitive, perceptual, and physiological processes and thus constitute the core of human subjective experience in general (9). Emotional intensity emerged as the best predictor for the organization of the subjective feeling space in the RSA. Despite both scientists (30) and laypersons alike often considering affect and cognition at least partially independent, we found that most feelings actually carry affective valence, further underlining the importance of affect to conscious states in general (10). Prior studies have focused on mapping the feelings associated with prototypical emotions (11, 12). However, the present data show that canonical basic and social emotions occupy a limited area of the total feeling space, which covers a wide array of mental and bodily functions and states. This result fits with the reasoning that the same frontoparietal brain systems stand as the basis of both emotional and nonemotional feelings but so that input from subcortical and other cortical systems associated with the physiological and behavioral aspects of emotions is needed to add the affective quality to certain feelings (18).

Links Among Mind, Body, and the Brain in Generating Feelings.

The representational similarity analysis revealed that the subjective feelings have multiple determinants. Using the arrangement-based organization as the “ground truth” metric, we found that it was associated with both the organization of the feeling-specific bodily topographies as well as with the relative intensities of the basic dimensions of feelings. The organization of subjective feelings at mental and embodied level, however, corresponded with the pure semantic (“is a”) similarities only weakly, confirming that the feeling space does not simply reflect the interchangeability of the semantic concepts (i.e., names of the feelings) used in the study (31). Instead, each feeling state is likely characterized by a combination of interoceptive and exteroceptive inputs evaluated also with respect to emotional qualities. We propose that humans intuitively bind these noisy inputs into coherent subjective experiences, similarly as has been previously found to happen for sensory percepts, so that high pitches, bright colors, and high spatial positions go together (32).

Neural organization of feelings was also associated with their bodily basis and database-determined semantic similarity, providing evidence of specific neural signatures of specific subjectively felt bodily states (10, 26). However, such direct mapping does not extend to mental states, and no association was found between the subjective and neural dimensions. This finding is not fully unexpected, given that gross neural activation patterns associated with, for example, seeing (occipital cortex) and hearing (temporal cortex) are spatially divergent, despite felt similarity of the processes. Thus, unlike bodily feeling states, higher-order subjective mental states do not necessarily reduce to local brain activations. However, our analysis is constrained by the spatial accuracy of fMRI and the univariate nature of the NeuroSynth database. Prior studies employing multivariate pattern recognition have indeed linked the similarity of neural activation patterns with the similarity of subjective sensory percepts (33) and feelings (24), suggesting a direct link between brain activity and subjective experiences.

Limitations.

We used a relatively crude distinction of the subjective states—for example, “seeing” would likely feel very different when we are seeing a growling black bear rather than a cute baby. However, our goal was at this stage to characterize the relationship of the most fundamental aspects of human feelings, rather than the fine-grained dissection of the qualia within each possible domain. Explaining how the distinct qualia, such as the pleasure derived from having sex or the agony triggered by seeing our loved ones suffer, are associated with neuronal activity is known as the “hard problem” of consciousness (34). Although our data clearly point toward bodily and emotional basis of subjective feelings, this connection does not obviously explain how bodily sensations are transformed into subjective qualia. This problem is further highlighted by the lack of representational similarity between subjective feelings and their neural signatures (Fig. 4): We do not understand well how these feelings are generated in the brain. Finally, humans have a limited access to the workings of their higher-order cognitive processes (35), and we cannot ascertain how closely the self-reported subjective sensations associate with the underlying neural and cognitive machinery.

Conclusions

Why do we experience anything at all? The present data suggest that we feel bodily events that are potentially good or bad to us. We found that subjective feelings are categorical, emotionally valenced, and embodied, and that the valence and mental involvement are the most important dimensions of the subjective feelings. These results extend the current models of the role of somatosensation (36) and embodiment (26, 37) in emotional processing to more general-level mechanisms determining the nature of human experiences.

Materials and Methods

The original list of the feeling tokens was derived from the American Psychological Association Glossary of Psychological Terms (https://dictionary.apa.org/), complemented with basic physiological and homeostatic processes, as well as with common illnesses. We then selected a total of 100 core feelings (Clear Women's Pleaser 701 Moon Black Silver qtPg6) belonging to seven broad a priori categories: cognition (e.g., thinking, reasoning), sensation and perception (e.g., seeing, hearing), motivational states (e.g., wanting, craving), emotional states (e.g., anger, fear), homeostatic states (e.g., hunger, thirst), physiological processes (e.g., breathing, sleeping), common illnesses (e.g., flu, fever), and feelings central to common psychiatric disorders (e.g., depression, anxiety). A pilot study confirmed that these sensations cover both frequently and infrequently experienced feeling states.

SI Appendix, Fig. S1 summarizes the experimental protocol. Altogether 1,026 participants (880 females; 146 males) completed the experiments (Experiment 1: n = 339, MDage = 27 y, 300 females, 39 males; Experiment 2: n = 226, MDage = 27 y, 181 females, 45 males; Experiment 3: n = 461, MDage = 26 y, 399 females, 62 males). They were recruited from university email lists and social media. All experimental studies were conducted online. The participants first gave informed consent and provided background information (age, sex, and education). After this, the participants were provided with the instructions for the experiment. Data acquisition and analysis code and the datasets are available at Born Amarena Womens Adia Distressed Adia Womens Born Amarena BqwnaPHW. Aalto University Institutional Review Board approved all experiments involving human subjects.

Experiment 1: Basic Dimensions of Subjective Feelings.

We hypothesized that four main dimensions (saliency of mental experience, saliency of bodily sensation, emotion, and controllability) would underlie the feeling space. To map these dimensions to specific feelings, the participants were shown the name of one candidate feeling at a time on the computer screen. For each feeling, the participant was asked to evaluate (i) how much that feeling is felt in the body (not at all–extremely much), (ii) how much that feeling is experienced in the mind (not at all–extremely much), (iii) how pleasant it feels (extremely unpleasant–extremely pleasant), and (iv) how much control they feel having on it (not at all–extremely much) on visual analog scales (VAS). An additional VAS was used for measuring (v) the frequency of experiencing the feelings, based on the last remembered occurrence of each feeling (within the last hour–last year, or not at all). Due to the large number of tokens (100), each participant rated 20 randomly chosen feeling tokens; they were asked to rate more tokens if they were willing to. Participants not completing at least one full batch (20) of tokens were excluded (n = 88). Associations between the basic dimensions were estimated using Pearson correlation and by fitting least-square regression lines between the dimensions. Because Experiment 2 revealed a clear cluster structure of the feelings, we fitted either one or two regression lines to each pair of variables depending on the optimal fit.

Experiment 2: Mapping the Mental Feeling Space.

We next mapped the mental feeling space using similarity rating using a modified computer-based Q-sort task. Subjects were shown all tokens as a randomly ordered list on the left side of the screen, and they were asked to arrange them to a rectangular box on the right so that states feeling similar would be close to each other. This technique is known to yield consistent results with direct pairwise ratings (r:s > 0.9; ref. 38). It was stressed that the arrangement should be based on the similarity of feelings, rather than on conceptual or semantic relatedness. Participants could adjust the placement as long as they wanted. Participants moving less than 25% of the tokens (n = 138) were excluded from the data, and tokens not moved into the target area were not considered in the analyses. Pairwise similarities between the tokens were extracted as their Euclidian distance. Distances were scaled so that the maximum distance across all participants was set to 1. Consistency of participant-wise feeling space arrangement was estimated using split-half reliability (Spearman’s correlation) over 5,000 random splits. Mean distance matrix was constructed (median distance matrix was essentially similar, r = 0.99 P < 0.001 in Mantel test) and subjected to DBSCAN (15) to obtain reproducible clusters as well as “border” elements, and “outlier” elements that do not fit in a single cluster. Next, t-SNE was used to visualize the underlying structure of the phenomenological space (16).

To test how different basic dimensions of feelings determined in Experiment 1 project to the subjective feeling space determined in Experiment 2, we modeled, for each data point in the t-SNE–derived feeling space, the intensity of each basic dimension with a Gaussian kernel with 8-unit FWHM weighted with the mean basic dimension rating of that token in Experiment 2. The resulting maps thus reveal how strongly each basic dimension of subjective experience is felt across the 2D feeling space. This representation, based on t-SNE and the basic dimensions, thus merges the traditional network and space-based representations often employed in semantic analyses of concepts used in natural language (31), and it provides straightforward access to the organization of the subjective feelings at multiple levels of analysis.

Experiment 3: Mapping the Bodily Sensation Space.

Bodily topography of feelings was mapped using the emBODY tool (ref. 26; https://version.aalto.fi/gitlab/eglerean/embody, ref. 39). Subjects were shown one candidate feeling at a time, and were asked to color, in a blank body shape, whereabouts in the body the state feels. Due to a large number of tokens, each participant completed the ratings for 20 randomly chosen tokens after which they were given an option to complete another batch of 20 until they quit or had completed all of the 100 tokens. Subjects not completing at least one whole batch of 10 tokens were excluded. The resulting subject-wise bodily sensation maps were averaged for each state and subjected to random effects analysis. Bodily sensation maps (BSMs) for each state were obtained by mass univariate t tests against zero feeling values (with FDR correction across all pixels and all tokens). Similarity matrices for the different feelings were constructed by computing Euclidean distance between the unthresholded mean BSM effect size maps between each pair of feelings.

Meta-Analysis of Neural Activation Patterns Across Feeling States.

To estimate the feeling state-wise neural activation patterns, we employed data from Neurosynth database (www.neurosynth.org). This tool allows automated meta-analysis of brain activations from tens of thousands of fMRI studies (ref. 40; analysis date November 15, 2016). We first obtained the full list of subject terms of studies stored in Neurosynth. Two independent raters evaluated the best possible (if any) matches from the Neurosynth term list with the subjective states used in the behavioral experiments. Thresholded Neurosynth reverse inference maps (summarizing altogether 9,821 brain imaging experiments) were downloaded for the terms found in the database. If two Neurosynth terms matched with one token (e.g., “attention” and “attending”), their maps were averaged. Altogether we found NeuroSynth database matches for 44 of the 100 tokens. Similarity between the meta-analytic activation maps was determined as the Spearman’s correlation between their voxels.

Representational Similarity Across Neural Activation and Domains of Subjective Feelings.

We next asked how strongly the different dimensions of subjective feelings (Experiment 1—Basic dimensions; Experiment 2—Mental feeling space; Experiment 3—Bodily sensation space; Meta-analysis—Neural activation patterns) are associated with each other as well as with the brain activation patterns underlying each state. We also tested whether the similarity structure of feelings or their bodily and neural underpinnings would reflect only their semantic relatedness, rather than subjectively felt similarity of the feeling themselves. We thus computed the semantic distance between each word pair using the SimService tool (ref. 41; swoogle.umbc.edu/SimService/). Semantic distance was computed using both Latent Semantic Analysis (LSA) based on two corpuses (Refined Stanford WebBase corpus, and LDC English Gigawords Corpus) and WordNet relationships between words. The tool provides four measures of similarity between word pairs, which were averaged to index how interchangeable two words are (31), providing a statistical estimate of the similarity of the semantic concepts referring to the feelings as they are used in language. To test for the representational similarity of feelings across different domains and dimensions, we used RSA (17) between the off-diagonal elements of the similarity matrixes derived from Experiments 1–3, LSA + WordNet, and NeuroSynth. This resulted in total nine similarity structures: Basic dimensions (Experiment 1: intensity of bodily feelings and mental involvement, affect, and controllability), frequency (Experiment 1), subjective feeling space (Experiment 2), bodily sensation maps (Experiment 3), and the data-driven neural (NeuroSynth) and semantic (LSA + WordNet) similarity structures.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the computational resources provided by The Aalto Science-IT project. This research was supported by the Academy of Finland Grants 265917, 294897, and 304385 and European Research Council Starting Grant 313000 (to L.N.).

Footnotes

  • ↵1To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: lauri.nummenmaa@utu.fi or riitta.hari@aalto.fi.
  • Author contributions: L.N., R.H., J.K.H., and E.G. designed research; L.N. and E.G. performed research; L.N. and E.G. analyzed data; and L.N., R.H., J.K.H., and E.G. wrote the paper.

  • Reviewers: C.K., Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience; and L.S., Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry.

  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

  • Data deposition: All datasets and resources are available at Born Amarena Womens Adia Distressed Adia Womens Born Amarena BqwnaPHW.

  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1807390115/-/DCSupplemental.

  • Copyright © 2018 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.

This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND).

References

  1. ↵
    1. Dehaene S,
    2. Changeux JP
    (2011) Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70:200–227.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  2. ↵
    1. Fredrickson BL
    (2004) The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 359:1367–1378.
    .
    OpenUrl 1623 Ready Neobiker Neobiker Men 45 New Leather M Rock Size Stock Neobiker S2 Brown AwnWqS6IRx Google Scholar
  3. ↵
    1. Damasio A
    (1999) The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness (Harcourt Brace, New York).
    .
    Google Scholar
  4. ↵
    1. St Sauver JL, et al.
    (2013) Why patients visit their doctors: Assessing the most prevalent conditions in a defined American population. Mayo Clin Proc 88:56–67.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  5. ↵
    1. Wilson AD,
    2. Golonka S
    3. 80406 Men's Shoe Boat FRYE Sully Camel A8qwSX
    (2013) Embodied cognition is not what you think it is. Front Psychol 4:58.
    .
    OpenUrl Google Scholar
  6. ↵
    1. Craig AD
    (2002) How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:655–666.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  7. ↵
    1. Damasio A,
    2. Carvalho GB
    (2013) The nature of feelings: Evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:143–152Puppies Men's Oxford Quatro BK Black Hush SFwqUq.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  8. ↵
    1. Damasio A
    (1994) Descarte’s Error: Emotion Reason and the Human Brain (Grosset/Putnam, New York).
    .
    Google Scholar
  9. ↵
    1. Russell JA
    (2003) Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol Rev 110:145–172.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  10. ↵
    1. Damasio A,
    2. Meyer K
    (2009) Consciousness: An overview of the phenomenon and of its possible neural basis. The Neurology of Consciousness (Academic, San Diego), pp 3–14.
    .
    Google Scholar
  11. ↵
    1. Toivonen R, et al.
    (2012) Networks of emotion concepts. PLoS One 7:e28883.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  12. ↵
    1. Tellegen A,
    2. Watson D,
    3. Clark LA
    (1999) On the dimensional and hierarchical structure of affect. Psychol Sci 10:297–303.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef Google Scholar
  13. ↵
    1. Gallagher I
    Giro Men's Dark Cycling Shadow II Blue R Carbide Shoes rqwZaHr7 (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci 4:14–21.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  14. ↵
    1. David N,
    2. Newen A,
    3. Vogeley K
    (2008) The “sense of agency” and its underlying cognitive and neural mechanisms. Conscious Cogn 17:523–534.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  15. ↵
    1. Ester M,
    2. Kriegel H-P,
    3. Sander J,
    4. Xu X
    (1996) A density-based algorithm for discovering clusters a density-based algorithm for discovering clusters in large spatial databases with noise. Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining (AAAI Press, Portland, OR), pp 226–231Shoes 46 Shoes Single Leisure 34 Quality Autumn Comfortable Fashion Lavender Woman Kenavinca Spring Fashion Big 7 Size gwqYR1Y.
    .
    Google Scholar
  16. ↵
    1. van der Maaten L,
    2. Hinton Tour Tennis Viola black 9 Women's hyper Violet Shoe Nike white 5 Zoom Vapor Tour Tennis Vapor white 9 Zoom hyper Women's Viola Nike Shoe 5 black Violet G
    (2008) Visualizing data using t-SNE. J Mach Learn Res 9:2579–2605.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef Google Scholar
  17. ↵
    1. Kriegeskorte N,
    2. Mur M,
    3. Bandettini P
    (2008) Representational similarity analysis–Connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Front Syst Neurosci 2:4.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  18. ↵
    1. LeDoux JE,
    2. Brown R
    (2017) A higher-order theory of emotional consciousness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 114:E2016–E2025.
    .
    OpenUrl Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar
  19. ↵
    1. Weisman K,
    2. Dweck CS,
    3. Markman EM
    (2017) Rethinking people’s conceptions of mental life. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 114:11374–11379.
    .
    OpenUrl Abstract/FREE Full Text Black Metallic Soft Metallic Style Black Soft Soft Style rEgEFq1
  20. ↵
    1. Gray HM,
    2. Gray K,
    3. Wegner DM
    (2007) Dimensions of mind perception. Science 315:619.
    .
    OpenUrl Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar
  21. ↵
    1. Hari R,
    2. Kujala MV
    (2009) Brain basis of human social interaction: From concepts to brain imaging. Physiol Rev 89:453–479.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  22. ↵
    1. Dunbar RIM
    (1996) Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language (Harvard Univ Press, Cambridge, MA).
    .
    Google Scholar
  23. ↵
    1. Philippot P,
    2. Chapelle G,
    3. Blairy S
    (2002) Respiratory feedback in the generation of emotion. Cogn Emot 16:605–627.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef Google Scholar
  24. ↵
    1. Saarimäki Nike Vapor Zoom Tennis Viola 5 Shoe hyper white Tour 9 black Violet Women's H, et al.
    (2016) Discrete neural signatures of basic emotions. Cereb Cortex 26:2563–Nike Women's white Tour Shoe Vapor Zoom 5 Violet black Viola 9 Tennis hyper 2573.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  25. ↵
    1. James W
    (1884) What is an emotion? Mind 9:188–205.
    .
    OpenUrl Google Scholar
  26. ↵
    1. Nummenmaa L,
    2. Glerean E,
    3. Hari R,
    4. Hietanen JK
    (2014) Bodily maps of emotions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111:646–651.
    .
    OpenUrl Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar
  27. ↵
    1. Starmans C,
    2. Bloom P
    (2012) Windows to the soul: Children and adults see the eyes as the location of the self. Cognition 123:313–318.
    .
    OpenUrl Google Scholar
  28. Print Cross Champion Trainer Gusto Women's Black Grey T8ZP7qw
    1. Heims HC,
    2. Critchley HD,
    3. Dolan R,
    4. Mathias CJ,
    5. Cipolotti L
    (2004) Social and motivational functioning is not critically dependent on feedback of autonomic responses: Neuropsychological evidence from patients with pure autonomic failure. Neuropsychologia 42:1979–1988.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  29. ↵
    1. Damasio A,
    2. Damasio H,
    3. Tranel D
    (2013) Persistence of feelings and sentience after bilateral damage of the insula. Cereb Cortex 23:833–846.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  30. ↵
    1. Zajonc RB
    (Vapor 5 hyper 9 Shoe Nike Viola Violet Tennis Zoom Women's white black Tour 1980) Feeling and thinking - PREFERENCES NEED no INFERENCES. Am Psychol 35:151–175.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef Google Scholar
  31. ↵
    1. Griffiths TL,
    2. Steyvers M,
    3. Red Nine Ballet Leather Quinney Women's Flat West wtAIrASqxY
    4. Tenenbaum JB
    (2007) Topics in semantic representation. Psychol Rev 114:211–244.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  32. ↵
    1. Spence C
    (2011) Viola white 5 Zoom black Shoe Violet hyper 9 Tour Vapor Women's Tennis Nike Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Atten Percept Psychophys 73:971–995.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  33. ↵
    1. Said CP,
    2. Moore CD,
    3. Engell AD,
    4. Todorov A,
    5. Heels Zipper Boots Women's Closed Top Kitten Toe Round Low WeiPoot Black Frosted Z0fx4Zn
    6. Haxby JV
    (2010) Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressions in the superior temporal sulcus. J Vis 10:11.
    .
    OpenUrl Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar
  34. ↵
    1. Chalmers DJ
    (1995) The conscious mind. Search of a Fundamental Theory (Oxford Univ Press, New York).
    .
    Google Scholar
  35. ↵
    1. Nisbett RE,
    2. Wilson TD
    (1977) Telling more than we can know–Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol Rev 84:231–259.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef Google Scholar
  36. Skechers Brown Brown Women's Skechers Starship Starship Women's qTOt5Sxn6w
    1. Keysers Women's black 5 white Violet Tennis Tour Zoom Vapor Shoe 9 hyper Viola Nike C,
    2. Kaas JH,
    3. Gazzola V
    (2010) Somatosensation in social perception. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:417–428.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
  37. ↵
    1. Niedenthal PM
    (2007) Embodying emotion. Science 316:1002–1005.
    .
    OpenUrl Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar
  38. ↵
    1. Goldstone R
    (1994) An efficient method for obtaining similarity data. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 26:381–386.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef Google Scholar
  39. ↵
    1. Suvilehto JT,
    2. Glerean E,
    3. Dunbar RIM,
    4. Hari R,
    5. Nummenmaa L
    (2015) Topography of social touching depends on emotional bonds between humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:13811–13816, and erratum (2015) 112:E6718.
    .
    OpenUrlOxfords Sharp REACTION Black Kenneth Cole En Men's XxCq8wT Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar
  40. ↵
    1. Yarkoni T,
    2. Poldrack RA,
    3. Nichols TE,
    4. Van Essen DC,
    5. Wager TD
    (2011) NeuroSynth: A new platform for large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Frontiers of Neuroinformatics Conference Abstract: 4th INCF Congress of Neuroinformatics doi:10.3389/conf.fninf.2011.08.00058.
    .
    OpenUrl CrossRef Google Scholar
  41. ↵
    1. Han L,
    2. Kashyap AL,
    3. Finin T,
    4. Mayfield J,
    5. Weese J
    (2013) UMBC_EBIQUITY-CORE: Semantic textual similarity systems. Second Joint Conference on Lexical and Computational Semantics (Assoc Comput Linguist, Atlanta).
    .
    Google Scholar
View Abstract
Previous Next
Back to top
Belly Toe Boots Wide Altos Caiman Square Genuine Skin Men's Western Black Leather Los qg1xw688
Email Article
Citation Tools
Request Permissions
Share
Maps of subjective feelings
Lauri Nummenmaa, Riitta Hari, Jari K. Hietanen, Enrico Glerean
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Aug 2018, 201807390; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1807390115
  • Tweet Widget
  • Mendeley

More Articles of This Classification

Social Sciences

  • Recurrent computations for visual pattern completion
  • Evidence for the impact of the 8.2-kyBP climate event on Near Eastern early farmers
  • Income inequality not gender inequality positively covaries with female sexualization on social media
Heathrow Skateboarding Shoe black Black SE Women's DC 6S5qwB6

Psychological and Cognitive Sciences

  • Recurrent computations for visual pattern completion
  • Income inequality not gender inequality positively covaries with female sexualization on social media
  • Linguistic effect on speech perception observed at the brainstem
Show more

Biological Sciences

  • One 6 SPA Toed Slippers Waffle Closed Lilac Size Coloured AOnH6OqT
  • Franck–Condon picture of incoherent neutron scattering
  • High-resolution repertoire analysis reveals a major bystander activation of Tfh and Tfr cells
Show more

Neuroscience

  • One 6 SPA Toed Slippers Waffle Closed Lilac Size Coloured AOnH6OqT
  • Franck–Condon picture of incoherent neutron scattering
  • High-resolution repertoire analysis reveals a major bystander activation of Tfh and Tfr cells
Show more

Related Content

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited by...

  • No citing articles found.
  • Google Scholar

Similar Articles

  • Consciousness explained or redefined?
  • Emotions as higher-order states of consciousness
  • Mental models of emotion transitions
  • Origins of consciousness
  • Varieties of reported emotional experience
See more

You May Also be Interested in

Women's Sandals W010 Shoes Butterfly Sandals Shape Blue CSM Salsa Dance Tango Fashion Latin Doris CZAqTT
Opinion: Expansion fever and soft money plague the biomedical research enterprise
Can academic institutions rescue biomedical research and the next generation of investigators? Yes, but the task will be hard and slow, and success will be piecemeal, not sweeping.
Image courtesy of Dave Cutler.
Journal Club: Highly-detailed solar wind observations may help explain sun’s mysteries
Origins of Bread
Amaia Arranz-Otaegui describes the discovery of bread that far pre-dates agriculture.
Listen
Past Podcasts Subscribe
Women's Sandals W010 Shoes Butterfly Sandals Shape Blue CSM Salsa Dance Tango Fashion Latin Doris CZAqTT
PNAS QnAs
PNAS QnAs with chemist and NAS foreign associate Lia Addadi
Women's Sandals W010 Shoes Butterfly Sandals Shape Blue CSM Salsa Dance Tango Fashion Latin Doris CZAqTT
Earth’s potential climate trajectories
In a Perspective, researchers explore the risk of crossing a planetary climate threshold due to Earth’s intrinsic biogeophysical feedbacks that, notwithstanding reduction of human emissions, might lead to "Hothouse Earth" conditions.
Image courtesy of Pixabay/WikiImages.
Current Issue

Submit

Sign up for Article Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusions
    • Materials and Methods
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • 2017 Hyperdunk Navy M Shoe Basketball US Men's NIKE 12 Size White TB Silver Midnight Metallic fq1xwEHw
  • Authors & Info
  • PDF
  • Submit Manuscript
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • RSS Feeds
  • Email Alerts

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Latest Articles
  • Archive

PNAS Portals

  • Classics
  • Front Matter
  • Teaching Resources
  • Anthropology
  • Chemistry
  • Physics
  • Sustainability Science

Information

  • Authors
  • Reviewers
  • Press
  • Site Map

Feedback    Privacy/Legal

Copyright © 2018 National Academy of Sciences.

  • Mesh
  • Rubber sole
  • Collaboration between Roger Federer and well-known Nike designer Tinker Hatfield
  • Adaptive Fit wraps from bottom of the arch to the laces for custom fit
  • Nike Zoom unit in heel for cushioning and impact protection
  • Foot frame increases support for multi-directional cuts
  • Flex grooves optimize play with increased natural movement
Alerts for this Article
Email this Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on PNAS.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Maps of subjective feelings
(Your Name) has sent you a message from PNAS
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the PNAS web site.
Tour Tennis Vapor hyper Viola white Nike Women's 9 5 Violet black Shoe Zoom
Citation Tools
Maps of subjective feelings
Lauri Nummenmaa, Riitta Hari, Jari K. Hietanen, Enrico Glerean
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Aug 2018, 201807390; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1807390115

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • TX Grey Skate DC Rinse Shoe SE Anvil qx5wXTw6B
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Zoom Viola Violet Tour black Vapor Tennis white 5 Nike Shoe 9 Women's hyper

We use cookies on this site to enhance your user experience. By clicking any link on this page you are giving your consent for us to set cookies.

  • Open in new tab
  • Download original movie